There is continuing controversy about inter-ethnic differences
in IQ and why these might exist. Richard Lynn (Kanazawa, 2012; Richard Lynn, 1987)
proposed that Europeans and Asians are more intelligent than Africans because
during their evolutionary history they faced the challenge of surviving in
environments featuring extremely cold winters. He claimed that survival in cold
conditions would have created selection pressures for greater intelligence.
Africans on the other hand live in tropical conditions all year round and hence
did not need as much intelligence. Lynn (2006) has presented data
correlating the intelligence of different ethnic groups with the severity of
their winter climates. However, there are some anomalies in his data and the
theory itself is based on questionable assumptions.
The theory of cold winters proposes that survival in colder
climates poses two evolutionarily novel problems that would have required high
intelligence to solve: finding food and keeping warm (Kanazawa, 2012). Kanazawa (2012) explains Lynn's theory with fairly sweeping
statements about how easy it was to obtain food in Africa, whereas people in more
northerly latitudes had to rely more extensively on hunting, which presumably
required more intelligence. He cites a statement by Lynn “that hunting in the grasslands of Eurasia is more difficult
than hunting in the woodlands of Africa because the former does not provide
cover for the hunters.” This claim seems rather bizarre in light of the fact
that modern humans are thought to have evolved on the African savannah, that
is, open grasslands, and so African hunters would have therefore needed to
solve the problem of hunting without tree cover. Kanazawa (2012) goes on to
elaborate why Eurasian hunters were supposedly more sophisticated than their African
counterparts:
Effective hunting thus presents a whole host of new
adaptive problems for our ancestors in Eurasia to solve, including the
coordination of different hunters for a single goal and the manufacture and use
of hunting weapons. These problems were largely unencountered by their counterparts
left behind in sub-Saharan Africa. These novel adaptive problems exerted strong
selection pressures for higher intelligence.
Really? Does Kanazawa suppose that ancient African peoples
did not know how to coordinate hunting parties or manufacture and use hunting
weapons? Recent evidence indicates that humans have been hunting for at least two million years. Modern pygmies and Bushmen are known to hunt elephant and giraffe.
Would not hunting these large animals pose adaptive problems involving
coordination of hunting parties? Furthermore, some non-human carnivores, such
as lions and wolves, hunt in coordinated packs with admirable efficiency. Although
these animals are relatively intelligent I do not think anyone would seriously
suppose that they require the intellectual capacities of humans to perform
these feats.
Kanazawa argues that in cold climates producing fire is more
difficult because there are fewer natural brush fires where fire can be
obtained without making it. Also, in cold climates producing warm clothing and
adequate shelter is more difficult. But what evidence is there that early
humans needed a high level of intelligence to do these things? Neanderthals
lived in Ice Age conditions for thousands of years and therefore faced these
exact problems. Furthermore, Neanderthals were known to hunt European megafauna.
Somehow they learned how to coordinate hunting parties to kill very large, very
dangerous animals. Does this mean they
were more intelligent than the first modern humans living in Africa? I have
never heard any scientist seriously propose that this is the case. The
Neanderthal tool repertoire was much more limited than that of their Cro-Magnon
contemporaries yet they somehow managed to survive for millennia with rather
crude tools. Neanderthals do not seem to have developed any art forms, yet all
races of modern humans, even the ones Lynn considers the least intelligent have
developed art. Lynn (2006) acknowledges that anatomically modern humans first
appeared in Africa. Yet he does not explain why it is that the modern human
race that evolved in tropical Africa developed greater intelligence and
cognitive sophistication compared to their Neanderthal relatives who had been
surviving in harsh Ice Age conditions for so many millennia.
Lynn (2006) presents data on IQ, brain size, and winter
temperatures for a wide range of human racial groups to support his theory that
colder winters are associated with higher IQ and larger brains. However, he
does note anomalies in the data. The peoples of the Arctic endure the harshest
winter conditions of all. Lynn’s hypothesis would seem to predict that they
would have the highest intelligence and the largest brain size of any race. Lynn’s
review found that on average they did have larger brains than any other
race. He has argued that generally speaking, average brain size is correlated
with average IQ within a given human population. However, the median IQ of
Arctic peoples according to Lynn’s data is 91. This is within the normal range
but clearly not ‘superior’. Arctic people have been found to have unusually
strong visual memory that exceeds that of Europeans. Europeans who have
travelled with the Inuit have remarked upon their extraordinary ability to
traverse apparently featureless terrain and closely observe the smallest
landmarks and memorise their spatial locations. Lynn argued that this enhanced
visual memory is a result of natural selection in their Arctic environment.
Strong visual memory has also been noted among desert dwelling Australian
Aboriginals, where it may have been an adaptation to life in a desert
environment (Kearins, 1981). Lynn (2006, p. 144) argued
that high intelligence requires large populations to develop because mutations,
being chance events, are more likely to occur in large populations. Arctic
people have smaller populations than Asians or Europeans and therefore
mutations beneficial to intelligence did not occur. He argues that in Arctic
peoples some of their larger brain size may be devoted specifically to visual
memory. Strangely enough, he claims that Australian Aboriginals have smaller
brain sizes compared to most other races, yet like Arctic peoples some Aboriginal
tribes have apparently developed enhanced visual memory. To be fair, at least
one study reported that Australian Aboriginals had a larger right visual cortex
than Europeans, a part of the brain associated with spatial ability. He does
not explain why natural selection among Arctic peoples would result in larger
brain sizes or enhanced visual memory yet the same evolutionary pressures
associated with a cold environment would not also produce higher intelligence. Arctic
peoples have clear physical adaptations to the cold, such as short, stocky
bodies well-suited to conserving heat. Additionally, some scientists have
argued that a large brain is an adaptation to the cold that also helps to
conserve heat. Neanderthals are striking for having had larger average brain sizes
than modern humans, which has been argued to be an adaptation to the cold climate,
yet they were clearly less cognitively sophisticated than modern humans. His
argument about beneficial mutations occurring only in large populations seems
like nothing more than special pleading. Furthermore, he acknowledges that some
races, such as Pacific Islanders have smaller brains than Australian
Aboriginals, yet they have higher average IQs (Table 16.2).
When anatomically modern humans first appeared
in tropical Africa, more primitive hominids, such as Homo erectus, had been living throughout Eurasia for over a million
years. If cold winters were a stimulus to the development of greater
intelligence it is not clear why modern humans developed such high intelligence
in a tropical climate. Furthermore there is no compelling reason to suppose
that survival in cold climates actually requires higher intelligence than
survival in the tropics. It could be argued that survival in the tropics poses
special challenges that would require intelligence, such as coping with
tropical diseases and parasites. The explanation for why there are persisting
inter-ethnic differences in mean IQ scores remains unclear (Neisser et al., 1996). Richard Lynn
believes that these differences are due to evolved genetic differences between
distinct racial groups but this view is not widely accepted in academia. His
theory of cold winters as an explanation for this phenomenon does not seem at
all plausible.
© Scott McGreal. Please do not reproduce without
permission. Brief excerpts may be quoted as long as a link to the original
article is provided.
An updated version of this article appears on Psychology Today in my blog Unique - Like Everybody Else.
Other posts discussing intelligence
related topics
The
Illusory Theory of Multiple Intelligences – a critique of Howard Gardner’s
theory
References
Kanazawa, Satoshi (2012). The evolution of general intelligence. Personality and Individual Differences, 53 (2), 90-93 DOI: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.05.015
Kearins, J. M. (1981). Visual spatial memory in Australian
Aboriginal children of desert regions. Cognitive
Psychology, 13(3), 434-460. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(81)90017-7
Lynn, R. (1987). The intelligence of the Mongoloids: A
psychometric, evolutionary and neurological theory. Personality and Individual Differences, 8(6), 813-844. doi:
10.1016/0191-8869(87)90135-8
Lynn, R. (2006). Race differences in intelligence: an
evolutionary analysis: Washington Summit Publishers.
Neisser et al. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns and Unknowns
American Psychologist, 51
(2), 77-101 DOI: 10.1037//0003-066X.51.2.77
Living in cold climate is a challenge. You can survive a night in the tropics, you can not survive a night in a cold climate, unless you take some type of precaution. Huge selection pressure. People in colder climates have fewer children than people in tropical climates.
ReplyDeleteIn cold climates it is not about how many females you can knock up, it is about how many of your offspring actually survive. Every race has their specialities, cold climate people were selected to be able to solve the problems and challenges of the cold. Warm climate people have far superiour abilities in other areas, and not a lot of research looks at these abilities. But certainly some of the abilities of cold climate people show as measurable abilities in the modern world. So yes, I do believe cold climate selects for these abilities.
Neanderthal immediate ancestors were probably H erectus or related species like H heidelberg. Neanderthals have larger brains (1500cc) and possibly higher intelligence than their immediate ancestor tropical H erectus (900cc) so yes cold does select for bigger brains.
ReplyDeleteIt is possible an H erectus population evolved to H sapeins in Asian then colonized africa.
As Peter Frost observes, there is unlikely to be a single explanation for group differences. Ed Miller has noted that one consequence of the colder climate is in the importance of paternal provisioning ('Paternal Provisioning versus Mate Seeking in Human Populations' Personality and Individual Differences, Vol. 17, August 1994, No. 2, 227-255).
ReplyDeleteAlso, Harpending & Cochran have noted a similar distinction in cad/dad cultures. For example, in tropical environments where women do a greater proportion of the work (eg. female farming societies), are less dependent on male provisioning, there's greater polygamy, you get more selection for charismatic big men, or strong warriors. As Cochran & Harpending note:
"These different subsistence ecologies are not only products of evolution and natural selection, they are also important contexts for it. If belligerent violent warriors, killers, have a reproductive advantage as the do among the Yanomamo of the Amazon basin (Chagnon 1988) then over a time scale of centuries males will become better and better at being belligerent and violent. If instead the social system favors white collar skills, like the niche of northern European Jews in Medieval times, intelligence and other related skill will increase. Centuries of peasant farming should select for being a good peasant farmer and related abilities. A system with a high interest rate, social stability, and the rule of law should select for the ability to defer rewards, against violence, and in favor of the ability to work hard. Gregory Clark (2007) suggests that precisely these circumstances in Medieval Europe led to the Industrial Revolution, that evolution forged a new kind of human that could sustain an industrial society."
http://the10000yearexplosion.com/human-cultural-diversity/
Peter Frost discusses this also:
ReplyDelete"Recent selection
Thus, one theoretical model cannot account for all or even most variation in cognitive capacity among present-day humans. The higher IQ of East Asians, for example, almost certainly came about during historic times and was probably favored by the widespread use of exams as a means of social advancement. Likewise, the higher IQ of Ashkenazi Jews and other European populations is probably post-medieval in origin and driven by the high fertility of successful entrepreneurs, particularly those in cottage industries who could expand their workforces only by having larger families (Frost, 2007).
Another relevant factor is the rise of the State, particularly its monopoly on violence (Frost, 2010). This is discussed with respect to English society in Gregory Clark’s Farewell to Alms. Clark (2007) argues that the slow but steady demographic expansion of the English middle class from the 12th century onward gradually raised the population mean for predispositions to non-violence, deferment of pleasure, and other future-oriented behavior. Although the embryonic middle class was initially a small minority in medieval England, its descendants grew in number and gradually replaced the lower class through downward mobility. By the 1800s, its lineages accounted for most of the English population...
Today, most of the human gene pool is derived from populations that only 15,000 years ago were confined to the northern tier of Eurasia. These populations have since expanded southward into temperate and even tropical Eurasia, as well as Oceania and the Americas. In the process, they have displaced other populations that were nonetheless better adapted in terms of climate and ecology.
What was their competitive advantage? It could not have been a K-type reproductive strategy. If we look at present-day hunter-gatherers from the northern arctic and sub-arctic, we find that they pursue a moderately r-type strategy despite high levels of paternal investment. Traditional Inuit, for instance, have short inter-birth intervals, with menstruation being a rare occurrence.
The competitive advantage seems to involve three characteristics of ancestral northern Eurasians:
1. A predictable yearly cycle, which favored the ability to plan ahead and make future decisions in the present. Indeed, early modern humans had more complex tools and weapons at arctic latitudes than at tropical latitudes, apparently because of the yearly cycle of resource availability: “Technological complexity in colder environments seems to reflect the need for greater foraging efficiency in settings where many resources are available only for limited periods of time.” (Hoffecker, 2002, p. 135)
2. A low incidence of polygyny, which reduced male-male competition for mates and the consequent disruptive effects on social organization.
3. A high level of paternal investment in the family, which in turn emancipated women from food provisioning and enabled them to develop a ‘family workshop’ of garment making, structure building, food processing, etc. (Kelly 1995, p. 262-270).
These northern Eurasians were thus mentally pre-adapted, despite their simple social organization, for later technological developments, even though such developments were possible only in more southern environments for which these populations were less ecologically adapted. It is perhaps no surprise that they were able to expand southward into the temperate and tropical zones, eventually peopling almost all of Eurasia, Oceania, and the Americas."
http://evoandproud.blogspot.co.nz/2011/02/rethinking-intelligence-and-human.html
Another factor raised by Cochran is the high paternal age in polygamous societies. Over time this would increase the mutational load.
ReplyDelete"The poster-boy population for checking out the consequences of a long-term pattern of advanced paternal age has to be the Australian Aborigines. They are not the only population of interest – there are other cultural patterns that change average paternal age. Like polygamy.
Someone should have noticed this a long time ago. Weinberg noticed increasing levels of trouble with parental age 100 years ago. Haldane had figured out that most mutations were contributed by males in 1947. He also developed the theory of genetic load before 1960. The atypical paternal age of Australian Aborigines has been well-documented for a long time."
http://westhunt.wordpress.com/2012/09/05/obvious-yessss-it-was-obvious/