In a previous post, I discussed evidence for and against a general factor of personality (GFP). Existing theories of personality organise personality traits in a hierarchical structure, in which a small number of broad factors, say five or six, subsume a vast number of narrower traits. Some psychologists have proposed a higher order general factor that combines all the broad traits into one super-factor composed of all the socially desirable features of personality. According to one theory, the general factor of personality represents an evolved “slow” life history strategy associated with long-term mating as opposed to a "fast" strategy associated with short-term mating. However, a recent study suggests that both slow and fast life history strategies each combine mixtures of desirable and undesirable traits. The findings of this study might help explain not only why so many people have “dark personalities” embodying socially undesirable traits, but why these traits can actually attractive. The so-called general factor of personality might represent an imagined ideal that few people embody rather than a single underlying dimension of human personality variation.
The Dark Side has a strange allure for many people
Currently, the most widely accepted
model of personality traits model is the Big Five, which consists of neuroticism, extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness to experience, all of which are considered to be separate
and distinct from each other. A more
recent model which has become increasingly popular, the HEXACO, adds a sixth factor of honesty-humility to the
Big Five.[1] Although they disagree about the exact number, both of these models
agree that the top of the personality hierarchy consists of multiple and
distinct factors. However, some psychologists, have argued that these broad
factors are not actually independent and that there is higher order super-factor
atop the personality hierarchy that combines all of them into one (Musek, 2007). For example, Rushton and Irwin (2011)
argued that this general factor is a dimension of “good personality” as opposed
to a “difficult personality”, with desirable traits manifested at one end, e.g.
someone who is friendly, cooperative, relaxed, reliable, and clever compared to
someone who does not get along with others, and is selfish, manipulative,
irritable and dense. Studies on the GFP have found that it is positively
correlated with subjective well-being, self-esteem, trait emotional
intelligence, and even general
intelligence apparently. Perhaps, this combination of
traits should be called the “best” personality rather than merely “good”?
Rushton
and Irwing proposed that this general factor of personality reflects a single
broad dimension that has been selected for in human evolution they call the
K-factor. This K-factor supposedly applies to a whole range of human
characteristics that are said to have co-evolved, including altruism,
intelligence, attachment styles, growth, longevity, sexuality, and fecundity
and which “form a coherent whole” (Rushton &
Irwing, 2011). The idea of a K-factor is the basis for what is called
life history theory which looks at individual differences in human reproductive
strategies. According to this theory, people with a “slow” life history
strategy (characterised by a preference for long-term mating) exhibit a high
K-factor, whereas people with a “fast” life history strategy (characterised by
a preference for short-term mating and promiscuity) exhibit a low K-factor.
According to a number of studies, slow life history strategy
is associated with better mental and physical health and subjective well-being
and with greater relationship satisfaction. On the other, fast life history strategy has been linked with
socially undesirable characteristics, such as criminality and antisocial
behaviour including sexual coercion (Sherman,
Figueredo, & Funder, 2013). If this is true, then it would seem that
from an evolutionary standpoint the slow strategy is desirable in every way,
while the fast strategy is completely undesirable. This is problematic because
if one strategy is “better” in every way, the alternative strategy should have
died out long ago for failure to compete. However, the fact that so many people
still utilise a fast strategy suggests that it may be adaptive under some
circumstances.
In spite of the alleged global adaptive superiority of the
slow strategy, there is evidence that this strategy involves costs as well as
benefits and conversely that the fast strategy enjoys its own advantages, in
spite of its drawbacks. This is because socially desirable behaviours are
generally those that are good for other people but not necessarily oneself,
while socially undesirable behaviours inflict costs on other people rather than
on the self. Social norms then tend to favour behaviour that is closer to the
slow end of the continuum. Hence, even though the slow strategy is desirable
from the viewpoint of society, it is not always in the interests of the
individual. For example, being honest and altruistic benefits society but may
be costly to the individual. Conversely, lying and cheating are costly to
society but may benefit the individual, at least in the short term. The slow
strategy might be smarter in the long-term, but generally requires individuals
to make sacrifices for the good of others.
This might be taking the slow strategy a bit too far
Recently Sherman et al. (2013) tested the idea that the slow
and fast strategies respectively combine both adaptive and maladaptive traits. Previous
studies on life history strategy that found that the slow strategy was
associated with just about every benefit one could want have been based on
self-report measures of behaviour and personality. Similarly, most studies that
have been used to validate a GFP have relied on self-report as well. A problem
with self-report measures is that people’s responses may reflect evaluative
biases. Because the slow strategy is so socially normative, people’s responses
may be biased towards reporting what is considered “normal”. This could explain
to some extent why the slow strategy is supposed to be associated with physical
and mental health, considering that the latter are also normative. Sherman et
al.’s research therefore used studies based on direct observations of behaviour
as well as participants’ reports of their behaviour in the last 24 hours to
overcome some of the limitations of self-report measures. Trained raters were
asked to assess how closely individual participants matched a template for
either a slow or fast life history strategy based on assessments of their
behavior. The template for the slow pattern included qualities such as
responsible, warm, compassionate and capable of close relationships. The fast
template included qualities of unpredictable, deceitful, manipulative, and
non-conforming. The resulting pattern that emerged was that those who more
closely matched the slow template were described as kind, considerate, and hard
working, yet also socially awkward, insecure, shy, lacking expressiveness and
emotionally over-controlled. Those who more closely matched the fast template
were described as unpredictable, hostile, moody, manipulative and impulsive,
yet also talkative, socially skilled, dominant, assertive charming and
interesting.
What these results suggest is that both the slow and fast
strategies have their respective strengths and weaknesses. This is consistent
with the idea that each one may be adaptive under some circumstances, yet
maladaptive under others. On the other hand, the results appear to contradict
the notion that one strategy is globally better than the other. Furthermore, in
terms of personality traits expressed, neither strategy appears to fit in with
the notion of a general factor of personality which combines all socially
desirable traits in a uniform way. Participants who demonstrated a slow
strategy could be described as agreeable, conscientious, and honest, yet also
introverted and to a certain extent neurotic. On the other hand, those who demonstrated
a fast strategy showed the opposite pattern of disagreeableness, dishonesty,
and low conscientiousness, but were also more extraverted and emotionally
stable. The fast life history strategy also seems consistent with a group of
socially undesirable traits known as the “dark triad” of psychopathy,
Machiavellianism, and narcissism. One study found that people who are high in
“dark triad” traits tend to manifest a pattern of being selfish, disagreeable
and low in conscientiousness, yet also extraverted, confident and socially
dominant (Jonason, Li, & Teicher, 2010).
This particular pattern of traits may allow people to successfully exploit
others for selfish reasons and yet escape social punishment due to their social
skills and charms. The authors of this paper compared this personality
configuration to James Bond. Another real life example is the Italian
adventurer Casanova. This fascinating fellow, notorious for his many love
affairs, was noted as a sparkling conversationalist who stated that the chief
business of his life was cultivating sensory pleasure. He also admitted to
swindling people who he managed to convince that he had magical powers.
Why do bad boys have all the fun?
Researchers have argued that “dark
triad” traits may have evolved to facilitate short-term mating. Evidence for
this comes from a study which found that women rated men with dark triad traits
as having more attractive personalities than men who were low in these traits (Carter, Campbell, & Muncer). Another study
found that men who were high in psychopathic traits (one of the components of
the dark triad) were rated by female observers as being more physically
attractive than men who were low in these traits (Visser, Pozzebon, Bogaert, & Ashton, 2010). Perhaps, these
findings might help to explain why so many people are so fascinated by “dark” characters
both from fiction and real life. Casanova for example was not the most moral
person but he knew how to live in style!
What these findings suggest that the
traits associated with the slow life history strategy represent a “good”
personality in the traditional sense of being unselfish and of respecting society’s rules of
good behaviour but not in a global sense of being generally better implied by JP Rushton. However, people who follow a slow strategy seem to be less
socially skilled and may not experience as much immediate pleasure as their more
selfish fast strategy counterparts, who are more focused on having a good time,
often at the expense of other people. One of the differences that emerged
between the two strategies, is that people with the slow style appear
over-controlled and lacking expressiveness, whereas those with the fast style
are more lively and impulsive. This suggests that one of the key differences may
be in how much people inhibit expression of their impulses. Some people may be
overly concerned with not doing anything that might give offense to others,
whereas other people are more focused on expressing themselves, being less
anxious about what other people might think.
The findings from Sherman et al.
suggest that neither a fast nor a slow life history strategy is associated with
a complete set of desirable traits that a general factor of personality would
entail. In my previous post, I suggested that a general factor of personality
might not represent a unitary dimension underlying all personality traits, but
instead a particular cluster of separate traits combined in a way that
maximises a person’s well-being. Perhaps this entails a personality type that
can strike a balance between the conflicting demands of expressing the self on
the one hand and exercising the self-control needed to comply with social
expectations and rules for getting along with other people.
Footnote
[1] Another minor difference from the Big Five is that in the HEXACO model neuroticism is replaced with “emotionality”.
This article also appears on Psychology Today on
my blog Unique - Like Everybody Else.
© Scott McGreal. Please do not reproduce without permission. Brief excerpts may be quoted as long as a link to the original article is provided. Any version of this article appearing on sites other than Eye on Psych or my blog at Psychology Today has been ripped off without my consent.
Further reading:
What is an Intelligent Personality? - discusses the relationship between personality and various concepts of intelligence, particularly in regard to claims that a general factor of personality is correlated with general intelligence.
© Scott McGreal. Please do not reproduce without permission. Brief excerpts may be quoted as long as a link to the original article is provided. Any version of this article appearing on sites other than Eye on Psych or my blog at Psychology Today has been ripped off without my consent.
Further reading:
What is an Intelligent Personality? - discusses the relationship between personality and various concepts of intelligence, particularly in regard to claims that a general factor of personality is correlated with general intelligence.
Image Credits
Darth Vader by Dualspades at DeviantArt
Sacrifice poster created at http://diy.despair.com/ using image from Flickr
Sean Connery as James Bond from Wikia
References
Carter, G. L., Campbell, A. C., &
Muncer, S. The Dark Triad personality: Attractiveness to women. Personality and Individual Differences(0).
doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.08.021
Jonason, P. K., Li, N. P., & Teicher,
E. A. (2010). Who is James Bond? The Dark Triad as an Agentic Social Style. Individual Differences Research, 8(2),
111-120.
Musek, J. (2007). A general factor of
personality: Evidence for the Big One in the five-factor model. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(6),
1213-1233.
Rushton, J. P., & Irwing, P. (2011).
The General Factor of Personality: Normal and Abnormal. In T. Chamorro-Premuzic,
S. v. Stumm & A. Furnham (Eds.), The
Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Individual Differences ( First ed.): Blackwell
Publishing Ltd.
Sherman RA, Figueredo AJ, & Funder DC (2013). The behavioral correlates of overall and distinctive life history strategy. Journal of personality and social psychology, 105 (5), 873-88 PMID: 23915038
Visser, B. A., Pozzebon, J. A., Bogaert, A.
F., & Ashton, M. C. (2010). Psychopathy, sexual behavior, and esteem: It’s
different for girls. Personality and
Individual Differences, 48(7), 833-838. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.02.008
No comments:
Post a Comment